声音影响饮食行为:实证进展与理论构思
作者: 余习德 张小娟 / 13021次阅读 时间: 2017年5月13日
来源: 《心理科学进展》
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网参考文献

R;x`9p"z:XlAA0心理学空间D ZL [4vp

崔丽娟 , 张高产. (2004). 内隐联结测验 (IAT)研究回顾与展望. 心理科学, 27, 161–164. 心理学空间5k5}3[vL%v P

心理学空间"NP Er dD4FE R

黄宇鸿. (2007). 释"和""谐". 广西社会科学 , (5), 135–137.

&C;P6I fK-Jc!d0

g LYC8AsAX0王远坤. (2005). “声味相和 ”,一种有意味的形式 ——论中国歌舞艺术与饮食. 饮食文化研究, (3), 104–112. 心理学空间9k!]B2Z:f3@.C

心理学空间P };x OI O'i6s1S

叶浩生. (2014). “具身”涵义的理论辨析 . 心理学报 , 46, 1032–1042. 心理学空间Jj2Az)aDY/]V\

心理学空间3s$l2@ Y;uZ ruh

叶浩生. (2010). 具身认知 : 认知心理学的新取向 . 心理科学进展, 18, 705–710. 心理学空间P3I/YSd

心理学空间6p![ x,tbjv'~

叶浩生. (2011). 有关具身认知思潮的理论心理学思考 . 心理学报 , 43, 589–598.

)h`2t | O/c6~]Zge$o0

,H_'H-QY0周雯, 冯果. (2012).嗅知觉及其与情绪系统的交互 . 心理科学进展, 20, 2–9. 心理学空间v cb6NI,q,^t

:tz(x s @ s.N!br0Areni, C. S., & Kim, D. (1993). The influence of background music on shopping behavior: classical versus top-forty music in a wine store. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 336–340.

F\9v s:ty9^n0

/D\0WVns"w0Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 11818–11823. 心理学空间})s1Ia:p)|

心理学空间)Z"o@Ky-wJ g

Bronner K., Frieler, K., Bruhn, H., Hirt, R., & Piper, D. (2012). What is the sound of citrus? Research on the correspondences between the perception of sound and flavour. In C. Tsougras, P. Mavromatis, K. Pastiadis (Eds.), Proceedings of the ICMPC, ESCOM 2012 joint conference. Thessaloniki, Greece: ICMPC. 心理学空间[N/x Y#[(c)y%N

心理学空间 \"AV["\4L.n

Brown, R. L. (1958). Wrapper influence on the perception of freshness in bread. Journal of Applied Psychology, 42, 257–260. 心理学空间2fLq[ G J @A(p

F*Co'h'~F8p0Budinger, E., Heil, P., Hess, A., & Scheich, H. (2006). Multisensory processing via early cortical stages: Connections of the primary auditory cortical field with other sensory systems. Neuroscience, 143, 1065–1083.

q:d2|qsVFi}0

qw6yE@W0Budinger, E., Laszcz, A., Lison, H., Scheich, H., & Ohl, F. W. (2008). Non-sensory cortical and subcortical connections of the primary auditory cortex in Mongolian gerbils: Bottom-up and top-down processing of neuronal information via field AI. Brain Research, 1220, 2–32. 心理学空间Wy)U{B u a[.y

心理学空间a&Jo ^5X

Calvert, G. A., Spence, C., & Stein, B. E. (2004). The handbook of multisensory processes. Cambridge, MA, UK: MIT Press. 心理学空间k!Ue V|8Q.r&N$M

心理学空间s\:g6}!evgw7a

Carvalho, F. R., van Ee, R., Rychtarikova, M., Touhafi, A., Steenhaut, K., & Persoone, D., … Leman, M. (2015). Does music influence the multisensory tasting experience?. Journal of Sensory Studies, 30, 404–412.

'_H0Tcg2Y0心理学空间S4s!kL?e

Carvalho, F. R., Wang, Q., van Ee, R., & Spence, C. (2016). The influence of soundscapes on the perception and evaluation of beers. Food Quality and Preference, 52, 32–

&Z M!J.I WT*Ue(W)z0

/kqSs&T041.

Y+WRcQ$q0心理学空间)bu.c w2Vd/w

Christensen, C. M., & Vickers, Z. M. (1981). Relationships of chewing sounds to judgments of food crispness. Journal of Food Science, 46, 574–578.

!Z L'u-Zlu0心理学空间d-SqC*j#s/R3Q-tF

Crisinel, A. S., Cosser, S., King, S., Jones, R., Petrie, J., & Spence, C. (2012). A bittersweet symphony: Systematically modulating the taste of food by changing the sonic properties of the soundtrack playing in the background. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 201–204. 心理学空间o7k.H8A/E?;j,S4Ok S

心理学空间G?7T JUHf"G^

Crisinel, A. S., Jacquier, C., Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Composing with cross-modal correspondences: Music and odors in concert. Chemosensory Perception, 6, 45–52. 心理学空间N%d5Z6T| h

T9^NA fa0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters, 464, 39–42. 心理学空间7M"Jb5vt l{

| @n;hL8c)a0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010a). A sweet sound? Food names reveal implicit associations between taste and pitch. Perception, 39, 417–425.

Wq(eh-HpXb5H0心理学空间'kd1g+a3`#c

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010b). As bitter as a trombone: Synesthetic correspondences in nonsynesthetes between tastes/flavors and musical notes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1994–2002.

t5??1b&{0

*JZp K4p(^'_6rBH3Y0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal associations between flavoured milk solutions and musical notes. Acta Psychologica, 138, 155–161.

,k yjq*A6C1y+X(a p0心理学空间 v#n?h6B\

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2012a). A Fruity Note: Crossmodal associations between odors and musical notes. Chemical Senses, 37, 151–158.

(?8A/y3tX$}#L0

#}tI!jtZk0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2012b). The impact of pleasantness ratings on crossmodal associations between food samples and musical notes. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 136–140. 心理学空间 IX:C}Q

5?$O R!ZG |'_4q1\0Cummings, A., .eponien., R., Koyama, A., Saygin, A. P., Townsend, J., & Dick, F. (2006). Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds. Brain Research, 1115, 92–107. 心理学空间1}:Z5T0fz+YK

v8z%P?2P0O5AC0Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. The Making of Consciousness Harcourt Brace, 2, 81–91.

H$[ INo;S0

\2L\!x"rO Q)`a0Deadwyler, S. A., Foster, T. C., & Hampson, R. E. (1987). Processing of sensory information in the hippocampus. CRC Critical Reviews in Clinical Neurobiology, 2, 335. 心理学空间3Rkl8PJ Z[

心理学空间"d7pl N$E

Delwiche, J. F. (2003). Attributes believed to impact flavor: An opinion survey. Journal of Sensory Studies, 18, 347–352. 心理学空间{-A `.o GiQ

心理学空间9Z&d.z.t*iB B;LkZBW

Demattè, M. L., Pojer, N., Endrizzi, I., Corollaro, M. L., Betta, M., Aprea, E., …Gasperi, F. (2014). Effects of the sound of the bite on apple perceived crispness and hardness. Food Quality and Preference, 38, 58–64.

qCT/XaI5?0

ImD$d,?8L\*f0Dijksterhuis, G., Boucon, C., & Le Berre, E. (2014). Increasing saltiness perception through perceptual constancy created by expectation. Food Quality and Preference, 34, 24–28.

-Y`c6q,I-X0

wF'] I,{f W~A"]0Duizer, L. (2001). A review of acoustic research for studying the sensory perception of crisp, crunchy and crackly textures. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 12, 17–24.

EhrZ'^Q9^0心理学空间] ?*f/_"B5k| n

Ferber, C., & Cabanac, M. (1987). Influence of noise on gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet or salt. Appetite, 8, 229–235.

C eJQ S:U~5c9amj(l0心理学空间5J;BomKaw I5I+V%{

Fiegel, A., Meullenet, J. F., Harrington, R. J., Humble, R., & Seo, H. S. (2014). Background music genre can modulate flavor pleasantness and overall impression of food stimuli. Appetite, 76, 144–152.

3Jq U/S _E4qE0

cav-c t1x0Forsyth, A., & Cloonan, M. (2008). Alco-pop? The use of popular music in Glasgow pubs. Popular Music and Society, 31, 57–78. 心理学空间#g#v0n j]d,J

心理学空间d(E^&K~ e.vqv-v

Fusar-Poli, P., & Stanghellini, G. (2009). Maurice merleau-ponty and the "embodied subjectivity" (1908–61). Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 23, 91–93. 心理学空间 k$h'T,V,r~9?TZ(R;VG

3Byt.ph4WYZ0Galindo, M. M., Schneider, N. Y., St.hler, F., T.le, J., & Meyerhof, W. (2012). Taste preferences. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, 108, 383– 心理学空间#P5y\e%E$y*T3NS7rC!J

心理学空间9F:J'u^,SGgI

426.

)y\'^5x$H9nh BQc0

"jxh']L{0Grabenhorst, F., & Rolls, E. T. (2008). Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 723–729.

Fl:GB%t WU.m0心理学空间4PK2Acunl

Guéguen, N., Jacob, C., Le Guellec, H., Morineau, T., & Lourel, M. (2008). Sound level of environmental music and drinking behavior: A field experiment with beer drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 1795–1798.

0Q)?&o.Yc:O)^(K0

l4z)U$hG0Guéguen, N., Le Guellec, H., & Jacob, C. (2004). Sound level of background music and alcohol consumption: an empirical evaluation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 34– 心理学空间r1lS-m-Si0i dK

{6Pi r(u;E0D5~038.

l)BE#eM4RPo'V:{_0C0心理学空间\h6q8{[5j!v_Ya

Ikemoto, S. (2007). Dopamine reward circuitry: Two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens–olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Research Reviews, 56, 27–78.

v V@y c?0

Owg] bb0Kejr, A., Gigante, C., Hames, V., Krieg, C., Mages, J., K.nig, N., … Diel F. (2010). Receptive music therapy and salivary histamine secretion. Inflammation Research, 59, 217–218.

t|)Di\"}b#w5_0心理学空间'P*F7q@y0y$bgD sa

Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Music, language and meaning: Brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 302–307.

:Y`z@os0心理学空间)M)pnj(E+]r

Koelsch, S. (2012). Brain and music. Oxford, England: John 心理学空间(fa:^P`'Z

心理学空间Z*R6E I7|L#zI

Wiley & Sons. 心理学空间)E~PT EzU

心理学空间5?2D&|d6m&K

Kontukoski, M., Luomala, H., Mesz, B., Sigman, M., Trevisan, M., Rotola-Pukkila, M., & Hopia A. I. (2015). Sweet and sour: Music and taste associations. Nutrition & Food Science, 45, 357–376. 心理学空间o0H1m4|:a

uF)Z(o/hI}0Kn.ferle, K. (2012). Using customer insights to improve product sound design. Marketing Review St. Gallen, 29, 47–53. 心理学空间_.IA)q w H }*]

"~}3Mg(u6gs2BF?0Kn.ferle, K., & Spence, C. (2012). Crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 992–1006.

WS(xX$[w:ou0心理学空间-gJeE"P+[ P%a9L

Kn.ferle, K. M., Woods, A., K.ppler, F., & Spence, C. (2015). That sounds sweet: Using cross-modal correspondences to communicate gustatory attributes. Psychology & Marketing, 32, 107–120. 心理学空间4A J T_*Hq J

Y*e|CP!Z%}%h9y9Z0Kupfkrmann, I. (1964). Eating behaviour induced by sounds. Nature, 201, 324.

]7VVr V^2AI`0心理学空间8|:HX u1gJ%I4Pc

Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 819–831. 心理学空间r;t`eX0GZ

"?"n!tG[j5i#e0Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

,C*_v lf3O0

;M/j]8O)bo-t6Vq0McCarron, A., & Tierney, K. J. (1989). The effect of auditory stimulation on the consumption of soft drinks. Appetite, 13, 155–159. 心理学空间&G0d6u"tZ,x E

心理学空间6v;stdO"WWVd

Mesz, B., Sigman, M., & Trevisan, M. A. (2012). A composition algorithm based on crossmodal taste-music correspondences. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 71. 心理学空间L;d7Z1I Kq:U7pg^

心理学空间r8w,M+V5a*U/T3m

Mesz, B., Trevisan, M. A., & Sigman, M. (2011). The taste of music. Perception, 40, 209–219.

|FE xZIrs;B*r+W0

*p9VgN]X9Vm W0Milliman, R. E. (1986). The influence of background music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 286–289.

U/G A:HXUv0

zM~V-ZVe4hj0Newquist, G., & Gardner, R. A. (2015). Reconsidering food reward, brain stimulation, and dopamine: Incentives act forward. The American Journal of Psychology, 128, 431–444. 心理学空间sL'Olv.F5Q

ai9\2KT0North, A. C. (2012). The effect of background music on the taste of wine. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 293– 心理学空间Z$|&m)qMn

eVd`:z(e0301. North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & Mckendrick, J. (1997). In-store music affects product choice. Nature, 390, 132. 心理学空间"}'Q6Q8\J8P3`D/J

心理学空间a+\%p'@ _"m NTe!yD

North, A. C., Sheridan, L. P., & Areni, C. S. (2016). Music congruity effects on product memory, perception, and choice. Journal of Retailing, 92, 83–95. 心理学空间kq/e_r2}`

心理学空间$] @!Tl,cn9M

Patel, A. D., & Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: The action simulation for auditory prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. 心理学空间l]~ W/f$Em(D5e.lJ

:F k.l#L!C/e4i0心理学空间fKB7G/S

T S;@p t f8t0Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 57. 心理学空间J7@'a ee;?x[Iq6\

心理学空间4wea*^1z,yG J

Plailly, J., Howard, J. D., Gitelman, D. R., & Gottfried, J. A. (2008). Attention to odor modulates thalamocortical connectivity in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 5257–5267.

/e]dt'z){%a:^0心理学空间W.YX,P4Ob)YO:S5h

Plotto, A., Margaría, C. A., Goodner, K. L., Goodrich, R., & Baldwin, E. A. (2004). Odour and flavour thresholds for key aroma components in an orange juice matrix: Terpenes and aldehydes. Flavour and Fragrance Journal, 19, 491–498.

)~'^ R7K0`4nK1xH0心理学空间}Fg.Y+H7]

Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 284–294. 心理学空间Lzq hS%N5G-\s

心理学空间@o,a C~(M R

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 257–262.

)Z#nX6D;L/dK0心理学空间,_j^s%z8M

Seo, H. S., H.hner, A., Gudziol, V., Scheibe, M., & Hummel,

O;@G)U/q6}Jdzr0心理学空间V^haU5OHW

T. (2012). Influence of background noise on the performance in the odor sensitivity task: Effects of noise type and extraversion. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 89–97.

D9l'og2[ GqW0

D0V%h0uN6ql H$g0Seo, H. S., & Hummel, T. (2011). Auditory–olfactory integration: Congruent or pleasant sounds amplify odor pleasantness. Chemical Senses, 36, 301–309.

oiO.S4^0心理学空间 _Vp0g[ CHFq

Seo, H. S., Lohse, F., Luckett, C. R., & Hummel, T. (2014). Congruent sound can modulate odor pleasantness. Chemical Senses, 39, 215–228.

N'ynLt0

zP$d/iPPt0Slocombe, B. G., Carmichael, D. A., & Simner, J. (2016). Cross-modal tactile–taste interactions in food evaluations. Neuropsychologia, 88, 58–64.

6k#w:nb XEv;r#z0

;O'b%o x%Rh1B0Spence, C. (2012). Auditory contributions to flavour perception and feeding behaviour. Physiology & Behavior, 107, 505–515. 心理学空间/gK(U`(Bs D4M&u:[

3V d/H!\ z*]S1L"H0Spence, C. (2015a). Eating with our ears: Assessing the importance of the sounds of consumption on our perception and enjoyment of multisensory flavour experiences. Flavour, 4, 3. 心理学空间2^V)u1\$\6I\)LE

心理学空间 Na(L uTF

Spence, C. (2015b). Multisensory flavor perception. Cell, 161, 24–35. 心理学空间9j9PW|8T D9B(a2J-k

2t%i(Z^ _6\lE0Spence, C. (2011). Mouth-watering: The influence of environmental and cognitive factors on salivation and gustatory/flavor perception. Journal of Texture Studies, 42, 157–171.

h`'o0V K|5JON0

bq}YTW0Spence, C. (2014). Noise and its impact on the perception of food and drink. Flavour, 3, 9. 心理学空间Nd0KD/C8AL

2jc"G h,H |.f/S0Spence, C., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2015). The perfect meal: The multisensory science of food and dining. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

]U7b DTC7{0

,G _W@m0Spence, C., & Shankar, M. U. (2010). The influence of auditory cues on the perception of, and responses to, food and drink. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 406–430. 心理学空间n%c,M @l&]

心理学空间0l6Ce1u {Y%Oi(a1s

Spence, C., & Wang, Q. (2015). Sensory expectations elicited by the sounds of opening the packaging and pouring a beverage. Flavour, 4, 35.

7w5Oxf q%a+b]0

[M4@;lR"\0Spence, C., & Zampini, M. (2006). Auditory contributions to multisensory product perception. Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 92, 1009–1025. 心理学空间#j~)F*q(]*v^W

zP*NiLY/c3P2G0Stafford, L. D., Fernandes, M., & Agobiani, E. (2012). Effects of noise and distraction on alcohol perception. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 218–224. 心理学空间ip1]'V'kL Y

zw(Y8i.v0Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press. 心理学空间 @ k z e I3L&g

心理学空间&l'WPz9f7U%Vb{0vC

Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2011). Affective priming effects of musical sounds on the processing of word meaning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 604–

Sa/a!J s2E~-\3L0心理学空间x MDC v9L

621. Stevenson, R. J. (2012). The role of attention in flavour perception. Flavour, 1, 2. Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2004). Effect of ambience on food intake and food choice. Nutrition, 20, 821–838.

`W%[ U5v8U4o;Hd!m3W0心理学空间4J*`A'~6h&K3JNc

Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2006). Listening to music while eating is related to increases in people's food intake and meal duration. Appetite, 47, 285–289. 心理学空间3v)[Q^(P!B(v

心理学空间1L]7f'yGav

van Aken, G. A. (2013). Acoustic emission measurement of rubbing and tapping contacts of skin and tongue surfaces in relation to tactile perception. Food Hydrocolloids, 31, 325–331. 心理学空间Sl6l} LDz_6R

心理学空间 WEHO J{1G zu8]

van Steenberghe, D., van der Glas, H. W., Grisar, P. R., & Vande Putte, K. M. (1981). The effect of acoustic masking on the silent period in the masseter electromyogram in man during sustained isometric contraction. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 21, 611–625. 心理学空间uzF.Y/E G["a5f3d

心理学空间,~Kml_X

Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., & Spence, C. (2014). Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Quality and Preference, 34, 88–95.

"yHH*G+Q$q N2I0

"K#X\hL0Walker, P. (2012). Cross-sensory correspondences and cross talk between dimensions of connotative meaning: visual angularity is hard, high-pitched, and bright. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1792–1809. 心理学空间~N qB:p x#d

心理学空间+Xw$IP@(_r

Wang, Q., & Spence, C. (2016). ‘Striking a sour note’: Assessing the influence of consonant and dissonant music on taste perception. Multisensory Research, 29, 195–208.

,~6IJjhO*m0

F1|%v5r/i y(CUH X-\w0Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C. (2015a). Assessing the effect of musical congruency on wine tasting in a live performance setting. i-Perception, 6, 2041669515593027.

8M3i9iUr] bR/F0

efxR+Qe"~S0Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C. (2015b). Assessing the influence of the multisensory atmosphere on the taste of vodka. Beverages, 1, 204–217. 心理学空间6lX5h0]h

[;h d eZ'\L A4`0Wesson, D. W., & Wilson, D. A. (2010). Smelling sounds: Olfactory-auditory sensory convergence in the olfactory 心理学空间3M8lwrQ%R$Nl%sb

心理学空间K mSC.GR yH

(OPxw!t(Xq!K0

,?d1nB{ K,cP0tubercle. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3013–3021. Yang, S. J., Gallo, D. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Embodied

Rm+Q5v)^[y0心理学空间:G"BKu!Pun&T

Woods, A. T., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., Poliakoff, E., memory judgments: A case of motor fluency. Journal of Dijksterhuis, G. B., & Thomas, A. (2011). Expected taste Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and intensity affects response to sweet drinks in primary taste Cognition, 35, 1359–1365. cortex. Neuroreport, 22, 365–369. Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2004). The role of auditory cues 心理学空间YC i&}y-?

心理学空间b3w]a1r"Z

Woods, A. T., Poliakoff, E., Lloyd, D. M., Dijksterhuis, G. B., in modulating the perceived crispness and staleness of & Thomas, A. (2010). Flavor expectation: The effect of potato chips. Journal of Sensory Studies, 19, 347–363. assuming homogeneity on drink perception. Chemosensory Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2005). Modifying the Perception, 3, 174–181. multisensory perception of a carbonated beverage using 心理学空间2Y&C;l-?/I8~-d5D t_

HC~1}Vu0Woods, A. T., Poliakoff, E., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., auditory cues. Food Quality and Preference, 16, 632–641. Hodson, R., Gonda, H., … Thomas, A. (2011). Effect of Zhong, C. B., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: background noise on food perception. Food Quality and Does social exclusion literally feel cold?. Psychological Preference, 22, 42–47. Science, 19, 838–842. 心理学空间T+V)G m\W"m7}~MG

心理学空间)Gq7G r'EZ:o'D-o ^

Sound influence eating or drinking behavior: Empirical progress and theoretical ideas

R:Z yXS#nX1C3~k0

ZE}|yG0YU Xide1; ZHANG Xiaojuan2; LU Cheng3; ZHU Yiyi1; GAO Dingguo1

[6WyoJ[(Ht0心理学空间9A!Im$B|ooTk

(1 Department of psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of 心理学空间?,a~x!g0F9gx

5A8ot)} iv0Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Mental Health, Guangzhou 510006, China) 心理学空间6LF%RzAN%P1p

eT%C,?5pRGFn0(2 Chaoyang Experimental School, Shantou 515100, China)

3D.z)io-O KBj0

0b e"rSP;h0(3 Nanfang College of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou 510970, China) 心理学空间#V#up-s}\;Na

心理学空间"DGX6SHnq |0vj

Abstract: Dietary behaviors consist of a series of psychological and behavior processes, such as sensing flavors, evaluating food’s and drink’s qualities or hedonic valuation, forming preferences for foods and drinks, as well as the explicit consuming actions and patterns. Dietary behaviors are influenced by sound, which, specifically, affects the sensitivity of the gustatory system and individuals’ food preferences. Sounds contain inter-oceptive cues and exter-oceptive cues that directly affect dietary behaviors. Inter-oceptive cues refer to sounds created when one interacts with foods, such as chewing sound, swallowing sound and food preparation sound etc.. Exter-oceptive cues refer to environmental or background sounds, such as music played in a restaurant. Studies have shown that the effects of sound on dietary behaviors are essentially cognitive, involving, for example attention, sensory integration (the match effect), and familiarity and expectation (the potential tone painting effect). The interaction between sound and dietary behaviors has also been studied by neuroscientists, who aim to uncover the underlying mechanisms, and from the embodied cognitive perspective. Both are expected to provide insights about dietary behaviors. Key words: noise; interactive sound; background music; dietary behavior; across-modal integration 心理学空间2@I P Fg#w`'r

www.psychspace.com心理学空间网
«自尊的认知神经机制 人格与社会心理学
《人格与社会心理学》
第18章 人类情绪的发生»