The Birth of High Gods
作者: Shariff / 9744次阅读 时间: 2015年6月25日
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

{ DO,U Xl3Rh0

:] ].}-Q|0References

BT.FrP8SKP\L0

9t3D'n]-qv@0Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. New York: Oxford University Press.

x?-P0E^iP0心理学空间%Qi]ReB"W7B

Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 713–770.心理学空间/g"f SuK-WuQ'P8xa Y

心理学空间Sr^ V}2@:[a

Atran, S., & Sperber, D. (1991). Learning without teaching. In L. Tolchinsky-Landsmann (Ed.), Culture, schooling and psychological development (pp. 39–55). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

%[/o4o;O"v_l$H,Q'B0心理学空间(_L6le D+lvh N

Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading non-natural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 69–100.

&l6G4l.w2?;Mq b,I0心理学空间&XxC*p4w!Tf

Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2, 412–414.心理学空间z["iP e6@/C@8XcS

6O*dk0z%G4XbM8C0Bering, J. M. (2003, August). On reading symbolic random events: Children’s causal reasoning about unexpected occurrences. Paper presented at the Psychological and Cognitive Foundations of Religiosity Conference, Atlanta, GA.心理学空间} w3k;XA^7u

心理学空间K7j6y'X.CE.} A

Bering, J. M. (2006). The folk psychology of souls. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 453–462.心理学空间]+HV,O8QyU

:j!Ts l tS;{l4Y&zd0Bloom, P. (2005). Is God an accident? Atlantic Monthly, 296(5), 105–112. Boesch, C., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39, 591–614.心理学空间?(}0E^$i)B

心理学空间e?uE c

Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(2), 125–143.

4nt/]pv9Z2W+cp0心理学空间!u0Y]6w$HW,J

Boyer, P. (1996). What makes anthropomorphism natural: Intuitive ontology and cultural representations. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.), 2, 83–97. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.

`a C f*| {C+ag0

&FQzC-l%qjH:j6J0S2O(U0Boyer, P., & Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science, 25, 535–564.

_s"Rso z}0心理学空间Xv KvlI"A

Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. New York: Oxford University Press.

6_$S#d-^l1j0心理学空间2Q oq"PVw0Z2RS pTT

Cauvin, J. (1999). The birth of the gods and the origins of agriculture (T. Watkins, Trans.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

-zy Vrw${Ue+BU5nO0心理学空间i9P f:C kR

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.心理学空间J&C5U:H4vU T

p:n-u/Ga4k0Durkheim, E. (1912/1995). The elementary forms of religious life. New York: Free Press.心理学空间8_^_L3e"z[[

心理学空间y3caqU,f-H|

Edgell, P., Gerteis, J., & Hartmann, D. (2006). Atheists as “other”: Moral boundaries and cultural membership in American society. American Sociological Review, 71(2), 211–239.心理学空间2[r1p)p(v-?.b4I'`Q

心理学空间5m;q-uwc\t^

Fodor, J.A. (1987). Modules, frames, fridgeons, sleeping dogs and the music of the spheres. In J. Garfield (Ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding (pp. 25-36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.心理学空间1O-im[ Y.K-p

心理学空间:y ]5n@3poI

Gervais, W., Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). Beware the infidel: The implicit mistrust of atheists. Unpublished manuscript.心理学空间yYR4M,p2f;m/Wb

7HR#wY8L]5]*ji0Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (in press). The moral mind: How 5 sets of innate moral intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules.心理学空间2~2| m,b~ []

心理学空间-oE}8HV8u4x

In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind (Vol. 3).心理学空间 g$l%w*li6sd7qT

心理学空间@ os]G/PLD

Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 245–256.

u$p:w)q/S6C!p_0

ZGo[ w-C9Bg0Harris, E., & McNamara, P. (2008). Is religiousness a biocultural adaptation? In J. Bulbulia, R. Sosis, E. Harris, R. Genet, C. Genet, & K. Wyman, (Eds.), The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques (pp. 79–85). Santa Margarita, CA: Collins Foundation Press.心理学空间7qL3B/r5JCS

心理学空间-mLX8C2XH5e

Henrich, J. (2006). Cooperation, punishment, and the evolution of human institutions. Science, 312, 60–61.

4V!jd_ ^0

o$jb-yn _R0?]0Henrich, J. (2007). The evolutionary links between cooperation, religion, and ritual. Unpublished manuscript.心理学空间:L4lx `C@R7U

心理学空间I&\z,_xX5o&b

Henrich, J. (in press). A cultural species. In M. Brown (Ed.), Explaining culture scientifically. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

;\Cc U f0

T7[5AV St(c"HI#nK0Henrich, J. & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–242.心理学空间 tf N3ho

#L$P(Wo!d,Z0Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 208, 79–89.心理学空间/go s y(E.x&s1RH

心理学空间@ Q{ `4[ _1lC6b

Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred status as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 1–32.

,y |2qkXxT#s"O8A!pQp0心理学空间t+FE\Ry3C5^BH

Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2006). Dual inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In R. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 555–570). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Y,F!u9Gk(j0心理学空间ErM&l` H[)v

Henrich, N. S., & Henrich, J. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.心理学空间UQ;w gneq};f

'r\&O Q Z0Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7, 346–380.

`cT?L@&WR*C0心理学空间;Wj/td VR

Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249–251.  

/D~7M,A#i| o#S,o0心理学空间"r,D+lXW

Irons, W. (1991). How did morality evolve? Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science, 26, 49–89.

:U5X(wC-I2jd0t0心理学空间L:G S)|G0r xn

Johnson, A. (2003). Families of the forest: Matsigenka Indians of the Peruvian Amazon. Berkeley: University of California Press.心理学空间RH4UyA1E$qF

1y(R{-T ~ F0Johnson, D.P. (2008). The supernatural punishment hypothesis. Oral presentation at The Society for Personality and Social Psychology Annual Meeting. Albuquerque, New Mexico. (February, 2008).

aiQt!MV~0

C Jt^%h mA#|2C5?0Johnson, D. D. P., & Bering, J. M. (2006). Hand of God, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 219–233.心理学空间BFm,pM;l

心理学空间&}gR$`j;V b(t x

Johnson, D. D. P., & Krueger, O. (2004). Supernatural punishment and the evolution of cooperation. Political Theology, 5(2), 159–176.心理学空间Gi M)l/LVb Wi/g

心理学空间5Y1E3E6m$Z*k

Kelly, R. L. (1995). The foraging spectrum: Diversity in hunter-gatherer lifeways. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Press.

(m Kb^5QHL5G0

n1S5z.nrX7I H0Kirkpatrick, L. (1999). Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion. Journal of Personality, 67, 921–952.

u0I%X8D"['@0

2^g/s7w;g!JG0Landau, M. J., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (2005). The motivational underpinnings of religion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 743–744.心理学空间;N-FC0l @1d

sf~|;jN0Leimar, O., & Hammerstein, P. (2001). Evolution of cooperation through indirect reciprocity. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 268(1468), 745–753.心理学空间:d NL@UeV v w

a|A-YeM%r y-F0McGrew, W. C. (1974). Tool use by wild chimpanzees feeding on driver ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 501–508.

l9z1r }"Od9rT0

zKo+g c-U+Mm0McNeill, W. H. (1991). The rise of the West: A history of the human community. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.心理学空间t Ye:qc|

心理学空间'k tO.mZr(w

Neudecker, S. (1989). Eye comouflage and false eyespots: Chaetodontid responses to predators. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 25, 143–157.

?1D?+pPY*a0心理学空间ey.~9w!xzA;Su

Norenzayan, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2006). Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science, 30, 531–553.

wm7~;Ok$Mf\v0

I4HbkBLP(Q0Norenzayan, A., Schaller, M., & Heine, S. (2006). Evolution and culture. In M. Schaller, J. Simpson, & D. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 343–366). New York: Psychology Press.

(\YEvYI0

[1k _ FN v'kCr0Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia.心理学空间#HYt,GJ ucS.^?@

心理学空间n$Ex7H I9Q.V5F

Nowak, M. A. (2006). Evolutionary dynamics: Exploring the equations of life. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.心理学空间b0UIa|$h)gI4hq

心理学空间 ]#stC |-?

Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 1291–1298.

[S9] QfRo)RsB0

7MqP%iHn's|2I0Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2003). A tale of two defectors: The importance of standing for the evolution of indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 224, 115–126.心理学空间-q6cgR;?;Y/~1s4XZ n

心理学空间'S Q:Oy ~/a

Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432, 499–502.

)b!Mx+dCu&g0

RgAFi#w0Panksepp, J., Panksepp, J.B. (2000). The seven sins of evolutionary psychology. Evolution and Cognition, 6, 108–131.心理学空间NzmF L^

rN(eC1lB4V0Price, D. T., & Brown, J. A. (Eds.). (1988). Prehistoric hunter gathers: The emergence of cultural complexity. New York: Academic Press.心理学空间(yZ#R$t^i

*ol0B2h6]v0Rendell, L. E., & Whiten, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–382.心理学空间t4~6OYIB#p

q|R7f j+Db!|0Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of ultrasociality. In I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt & F. K. Salter (Eds.), Indoctrinability, ideology and warfare (pp. 71–96). New York: Berghahn Books.

s;[` jK0O0

_a GhJ.b~/M0e0Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

5zLtn0s c0心理学空间Gc:j/q9S^]-k'K.x)Y

Rigaud, M. (1985). Secrets of voodoo (R. B. Cross, Trans.). San Francisco: City Light Books.  心理学空间_9XoI7N*V)P

0ihUej8_#xf0Roes, F., & Raymond, M. (2003). Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 126–135.心理学空间6J:c `t'uw0U^

O:Ytm0i5uu4_;Y0Rogers, A. A. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.心理学空间:s0J4?K8m5Eh@

o.?UV.stky0aH0Rozin, P. (1987). Psychobiological perspectives on food preferences and avoidances. In M. Harris & E. B. Ross (Eds.), Food and evolution: Toward a theory of human food habits (pp. 181–205). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

q&k-x'G U.g0

LNM!QM7I0Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–809.

xh'\mH'Z$i%a0心理学空间*]Sh.B1R)J1K-w5Kv

Sherman, P. W., & Billing, J. (1999). Darwinian gastronomy: Why we use spices. BioScience, 49, 453–463.心理学空间!pz)h^R.Z3H]/q!T|

/S-GLDJ~ G0Smith, R. J. (1996). Biology and body size in human evolution: Statistical inference misapplied. Current Anthropology, 37(3), 451–481.心理学空间^;]'I)jX#gTJ5x

心理学空间 s n!B"I2Y.X [2w;x `

Snarey, J. (1996). The natural environment’s impact upon religious ethics: A cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 80, 85–96.心理学空间+NRwo'B&W0Lz0g

心理学空间%`(?+R;AR o!i

Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. (2003). Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 264–274.

:?W;t&[)}FJ`;CB'\0心理学空间E3R9ssZszv^

Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. R. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37(2), 211–239.心理学空间t Zm'ts1eN-Og!W%Z

DO(W w?0Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2003). Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 44, 713–722.心理学空间_S#PCYk;}

心理学空间"jC#~k s Y6u

Stevens, M. (2005). The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. Biological Reviews, 80, 573–588.

$Wp`W6J/c1b'S0心理学空间/X1FMGF)Q!hn

Tan, J. H. W., & Vogel, C. (2005, September). Religion and trust: An experimental study. Paper presented to the annual congress of the International Association for Research in Economic Psychology, Prague.心理学空间V^@ QP3] r

心理学空间{/_.N'z:fL%`a

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.心理学空间8i/BRGqt

心理学空间6L3Im4n V-G_,R

Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–511.心理学空间TGr:J;g!WV z

心理学空间3|/^2y*F-j4^Z)Q.t,b

Tuzin, D. (1976). The Ilahita Arapesh. Berkeley: University of California Press. Tuzin, D. (2001). Social complexity in the making: A case study among the Arapesh of New Guinea. London: Routledge.心理学空间*G8D d8f Z)@

心理学空间pv^'q#`p i#I

Voltaire, F. M. A. D., & Redman, B. R. (1977). The portable Voltaire. New York: Penguin Books.心理学空间U,Uj,I:yq H

心理学空间q5eNnTI-a

Williams, G. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.心理学空间"K"nn/f\m

4^ Q)m;Q F'P0Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.心理学空间|+J#QB| Pj'Q

d]&gJ'W*L*V9V%L0Wrangham, R., & Conklin-Brittain, N. (2003). Cooking as a biological trait. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular and Integrative Physiology, 136, 35–46.

$cW*W{yt"K \0www.psychspace.com心理学空间网
«探究宗教与利社会行为之间关系 Ara Norenzayan 阿兰•洛伦萨杨
《Ara Norenzayan 阿兰•洛伦萨杨》
解构宗教与社会进化关系:大社会需要大神明»