Decoding the Brain’s Cacophony
作者: Michael S. Gazzaniga / 9113次阅读 时间: 2011年11月17日
来源: PROFILES IN SCIENCE 标签: Gazzaniga 认知神经科学
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网ST. HELENA, Calif. — The scientists exchanged one last look and held their breath.        
kS{,D!r0
E,E Z6ki)p\_\0心理学空间(_ cg2W Y_{

Michael Gazzaniga: An interview with the neuroscientist and professor of psychology known for his studies, and stories, about the brain's split personality.

.F0n7q-E:GTqn0

C%Z6U*sA0Michael S. Gazzaniga心理学空间5} E,h q)E3V?*E+n5}7E;i

This is the fourth in an occasional series of articles and videos about leaders in science.

n|Sa G4X7Rm4~ ^0
    

Michael S. Gazzaniga is a psychology professor at the University of California, Santa Barbara.心理学空间J6Q ^9F }'?1J9R

 
   

Michael Gazzaniga, in striped shirt, with his family in 1945.                            心理学空间)^!UB#P7z t4s$S/y

 
  

Dr. Gazzaniga as a student at Caltech in 1963.                            心理学空间E1l'W?(r!Sl+C4F3K

 

LEFT BRAIN, RIGHT BRAINDr. Gazzaniga with his colleagues John Sidtis and Jeffrey Holtzman, in lab coats, in the van they used at Dartmouth for studying the brain's hemispheric division of labor.                            心理学空间'\hSI9?%B(F*W

 

&^.g8Ehp0 Everything was ready. The electrode was in place, threaded between the two hemispheres of a living cat’s brain; the instruments were tuned to pick up the chatter passing from one half to the other. The only thing left was to listen for that electronic whisper, the brain’s own internal code.        心理学空间'P/X3r(li] U(AC/]

心理学空间-tv"v#j,[4b$T_

 The amplifier hissed — the three scientists expectantly leaning closer — and out it came, loud and clear.        心理学空间$M&Zg? j"CM_;y Z

+ku*TV\/m0 “We all live in a yellow submarine, yellow submarine, yellow submarine ....”        心理学空间E&U0j*nfxC

心理学空间Q"R:vY6V7V/E&@

 “The Beatles’ song! We somehow picked up the frequency of a radio station,” recalled Michael S. Gazzaniga, chuckling at the 45-year-old memory. “The brain’s secret code. Yeah, right!”        

-[x4h Qa+km1Y#s0心理学空间6B(|]$D3[^ A2l

 Dr. Gazzaniga, 71, now a professor of psychology at theUniversity of California, Santa Barbara, is best known for a dazzling series of studies that revealed the brain’s split personality, the division of labor between its left and right hemispheres. But he is perhaps next best known for telling stories, many of them about blown experiments, dumb questions and other blunders during his nearly half-century career at the top of his field.        心理学空间9s s)K_O:R#h

心理学空间KG ||+J`U~

 Now, in lectures and a new book, he is spelling out another kind of cautionary tale — a serious one, about the uses of neuroscience in society, particularly in the courtroom.        

)OgJ7aVf&^U'm0

8M O(^$yV%Vm] n*I0 Brain science “will eventually begin to influence how the public views justice and responsibility,” Dr. Gazzaniga said at a recent conference here sponsored by the Edge Foundation.        心理学空间bI'D N3m7gS

n$O`J3GW5^0 And there is no guarantee, he added, that its influence will be a good one.        心理学空间p"gj A&_)b{"D

S2b,DT(Z0 For one thing, brain-scanning technology is not ready for prime time in the legal system; it provides less information than people presume.        心理学空间qxa` _q/kk_

3JI$e2}L NNo0 For another, new knowledge about neural processes is raising important questions about human responsibility. Scientists now know that the brain runs largely on autopilot; it acts first and asks questions later, often explaining behavior after the fact. So if much of behavior is automatic, then how responsible are people for their actions?        心理学空间 O(T7Nv9Q c&a_f

e+MSB1i"U7q^ s j0 Who’s driving this submarine, anyway?        心理学空间h V9_.M5n5pe/u5{

C+xz)s9a"x0 In his new book, “Who’s in Charge? Free Will and the Science of the Brain,” being published this month by Ecco/HarperCollins, Dr. Gazzaniga (pronounced ga-ZAHN-a-ga) argues that the answer is hidden in plain sight. It’s a matter of knowing where to look.心理学空间2cF_6i{L }
心理学空间X?4ZMVH2_
       心理学空间P?"yv[LN

.y/_h-mn9t0 The Split Brain       

mM'_2VI6}[Y g.z0心理学空间 I6@r%gMhPt-^d!wM

 He began thinking seriously about the nature of responsibility only after many years of goofing off.        心理学空间9s f}e(B!Oc9O

心理学空间LHr9bL%hJ$c-w

 Mike Gazzaniga grew up in Glendale, Calif., exploring the open country east of Los Angeles and running occasional experiments in his garage, often with the help of his father, a prominent surgeon. It was fun; the experiments were real attempts to understand biochemistry; and even after joining the Alpha Delta Phi fraternity at Dartmouth (inspiration for the movie “Animal House”), he made time between parties and pranks to track who was doing what in his chosen field, brain science.        心理学空间M-gH-H6GW` fMX:~

Z_{]w4\I0 In particular, he began to follow studies at theCalifornia Institute of Technologysuggesting that in animals, developing nerve cells are coded to congregate in specific areas in the brain. This work was captivating for two reasons.        

^(]5b I$cN)a0

8cDc"JZG VY4b0 First, it seemed to contradict common wisdom at the time, which held that specific brain functions like memory were widely — and uniformly — distributed in the brain, not concentrated in discrete regions.        

iNn/k1Z]s6C0

h V7u9H&c,Hf z0 Second, his girlfriend was due to take a summer job right there near Caltech.        

_?}a%Vw+i ~'y0? o0

#@8Ft)Td {O&E&k4X0 He decided to write a letter to the director of the program, the eminent neurobiologist Roger Wolcott Sperry (emphasizing reason No. 1). Could Dr. Sperry use a summer intern? “He said sure,” Dr. Gazzaniga said. “I always tell students, ‘Go ahead and write directly to the person you want to study with; you just never know.’ ”        

.{x-G zl%z0

OkW2x9Xh4~)B0 At Caltech that summer after his junior year, he glimpsed his future. He learned about so-called split-brain patients, people with severe epilepsy who had surgery cutting the connections between their left and right hemispheres. The surgery drastically reduced seizures but seemed to leave people otherwise unaffected.        

bL9x4{ Lm_ Q.y+X0

8K.~ __([;Zy0 Back at Dartmouth, he couldn’t stop thinking about it: Totally unaffected? Combing the literature, he found that the best attempt to detect an effect had found no changes in thinking or perception among 26 patients who had had the surgery at the University of Rochester.        

P6fL{K!c0心理学空间#HFA'yW

 Could that be possible? Mr. Gazzaniga was so eager to test the patients’ perceptions himself that he wrote another letter, this time to the surgeon — and got permission to do so.        

5Tf6kD]:g3?7We0

E6s]Q jB'q0 “It’s spring break, I get all my gear together, I get all the way over there, and the guy changes his mind,” Dr. Gazzaniga said. “Like, ‘Hey, buddy, go home!’ ”        

ox0A,w%q$T-tN;r0心理学空间6_/V@l6E2Q:G

 After graduating, he headed straight for Caltech.        心理学空间kL9z%RZ9^G.D

0o z B'CpD A)U0 “It wasn’t just ambition, it was something else — he was gutsy,” said Mitch Glickstein, who was in Dr. Sperry’s lab at the time and is completing a book, “Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction.” “Here’s this junior in college, he knows all about the split-brain patients, and he’s ready to do original research. At 20 years old.”        

`4LB'k ``/A0

!z Erbcs u0 Caltech in those days was like a frat house forNobel Prizecontenders. Here’s Richard Feynman, the physicist, parking himself in the lab unannounced and making wisecracks about the experiments. There’s Dr. Sperry, annoyed, wondering how to one-up Dr. Feynman. One afternoon Dr. Sperry’s young student scrambled out into the hallway on all fours after an escaped lab animal and nearly kneecapped Linus Pauling, the eminent chemist. (“Why don’t you try anesthetizing a bowl of jelly instead?” Dr. Pauling remarked icily.)        心理学空间2Ol?7Z:@

l-qXA%R|0 And then there were the experiments, each one a snapshot into the dark box of the brain. In the early 1960s, Dr. Gazzaniga, then a graduate student, teamed with Dr. Sperry and Dr. Joseph Bogen, a brain surgeon, to publish a string of reports that dramatically demonstrated hemispheric specialization in humans.        

@7m@d9bs? P0心理学空间!V(oH[Q1j/a-R

 The researchers devised a way to flash a picture of a bicycle to the right hemisphere alone. When split-brain patients were asked what they saw, they replied, “Nothing”: Because of the severed connection, the left hemisphere, where language is centered, got no visual input and no information from the right hemisphere. So the right hemisphere — which “saw” the bike — had no language to name it.        心理学空间)|/aI{ M#O

4`qnUg2W`%x0 But here was the kicker: The right hemisphere could direct the hand it controls to draw the bicycle.        

lMn1R%] D0

8Q kMj-vV.uv0 In other studies, the three scientists showed that the right hemisphere could also identify objects by touch, correctly selecting, say, a toothbrush or a spoon by feel after seeing the image of one.        心理学空间7~ e] BIdL5D

"WGn2rZj9{3P0 The implications were soon clear. The left hemisphere was the intellectual, the wordsmith; it could be severed from the right without loss of I.Q. The right side was the artist, the visual-spatial expert.        

J5j7?#b { j(|0心理学空间+e/O};V1{9}

 The findings demolished the theory that specific functions were widely and uniformly supported in the brain. It also put “left brain/right brain” into the common language, as shorthand for types of skills and types of people. Still, in a field defined by incremental, often arcane advances, the Caltech team had achieved a moon shot.        

$x r4@'jdy0

?._EO Ag1s6y/B%aF,W0 Dr. Gazzaniga, now all of 25, could write his own ticket. He soon had a grant for a study to record the electronic chatter between the two hemispheres in the brain of a cat.        

W0E(v:Lw]&Y2F&v0

-W+h)A5H]0 The Interpreter       

KR,?F;@6Z7f0

8k mIUC!Z.}z0 The Beatles song that surged through the receiver in that experiment provided Dr. Gazzaniga with something almost as valuable as insight: a good story. Yet it also served as a rude reminder that he and his colleagues were missing something important in their assumptions about the brain.        心理学空间mX3o l(d(`

心理学空间%pgvc? u1i

 “The question, ultimately, was why?” Dr. Gazzaniga said. “Why, if we have these separate systems, is it that the brain has a sense of unity?”        心理学空间.L2z}`[,d~+C

心理学空间 ]-q u(]2Xu

 Even as he built his early triumph into a career, moving from Caltech to U.C. Santa Barbara and eventually to Dartmouth, with several stops along the way, the same question hung in the air, without a satisfactory answer. In the late 1970s, with the psychologist and linguist George A. Miller, he founded the field of cognitive neuroscience, a marriage of psychology and biology aimed at solving just such puzzles.        

%O BwP/M5l3[0

"|6~;aD(_0g7i0 It didn’t happen, at least not quickly. In the decades to follow, brain scientists found that the left brain-right brain split is only the most obvious division of labor; in fact, the brain contains a swarm of specialized modules, each performing a special skill — calculating a distance, parsing a voice tone — and all of them running at the same time, communicating in widely distributed networks, often across hemispheres.        心理学空间;QV+B%q:z%m%g5g:X

_ f,Do/ze^0 In short, the brain sustains a sense of unity not just in the presence of its left and right co-pilots. It does so amid a cacophony of competing voices, the neural equivalent of open outcry at the Chicago Board of Trade.        心理学空间StB+lw

心理学空间]'N"H"i#^

 How?        心理学空间?+|s{)QU

}CO;w.f&i4e0 It turned out, yet again, that people who’d had the split-brain surgery helped provide an answer. Dr. Gazzaniga, now at Dartmouth, performed more of his signature experiments — this time with an added twist. In one study, for instance, he and Joseph LeDoux, then a graduate student, showed a patient two pictures: The man’s left hemisphere saw a chicken claw; his right saw a snow scene. Afterward, the man chose the most appropriate matches from an array of pictures visible to both hemispheres. He chose a chicken to go with the claw, and a shovel to go with the snow. So far, so good.        

;^+as~3Gl^}q0心理学空间u.GTW`

 But then Dr. Gazzaniga asked him why he chose those items — and struck gold. The man had a ready answer for one choice: The chicken goes with the claw. His left hemisphere had seen the claw, after all. Yet it had not seen the picture of the snow, only the shovel. Looking down at the picture of the shovel, the man said, “And you need a shovel to clean out the chicken shed.”        心理学空间G ZxpcH@J!k

心理学空间%P F!BSre za

 The left hemisphere was just concocting an explanation, Dr. Gazzaniga said. In studies in the 1980s and ’90s, he and others showed that the pattern was consistent: The left hemisphere takes what information it has and delivers a coherent tale to conscious awareness. It happens continually in daily life, and most everyone has caught himself or herself in the act — overhearing a fragment of gossip, for instance, and filling in the blanks with assumptions.        心理学空间)kJy5~,l@

.^&K1T yIY0 The brain’s cacophony of competing voices feels coherent because some module or network somewhere in the left hemisphere is providing a running narration. “It only took me 25 years to ask the right question to figure it out,” Dr. Gazzaniga said.        

xb0v%Qv Dw4s0

c7jQ`(U { _n8}0 “One of the toughest things in any science, but especially in neuroscience, is to weed out the ideas that are really pleasing but unencumbered by truth,” said Thomas Carew, former president of the Society for Neuroscience and dean of the New York University School of Arts and Sciences. “Mike Gazzaniga is one of those in the field who’s been able to do that.”        

-g\p"c%r5W1A;}3y0心理学空间K/lCqu!m ?.a&B%k

 Dr. Gazzaniga decided to call the left-brain narrating system “the interpreter.” The storyteller found the storyteller.        

B x.j/J k]2bW,J2w0

(^3s{{3cU0 Emergent Properties       心理学空间 efj:y{B(V5l

心理学空间+w(_$af$[z5W,S

 Knowing the breed well, he also understood its power. The interpreter creates the illusion of a meaningful script, as well as a coherent self. Working on the fly, it furiously reconstructs not only what happened but why, inserting motives here, intentions there — based on limited, sometimes flawed information.        心理学空间vxY5tJO HC*H

心理学空间'N ^8`!y"` G8{

 One implication of this is a familiar staple of psychotherapy and literature: We are not who we think we are. We narrate our lives, shading every last detail, and even changing the script retrospectively, depending on the event, most of the time subconsciously. The storyteller never stops, except perhaps during deep sleep.        心理学空间*z7ud,ePh:m*oH

1S+y,a1U;ke-X0 But another implication has to do with responsibility. If our sense of control is built on an unreliable account from automatic brain processes, how much control do we really have? Are there thresholds of responsibility, for instance, that can be determined by studying neural circuits? Dr. Gazzaniga and his wife, Charlotte, raised six children, so like any parents they had to determine levels of responsibility on the fly, just to get someone to set the table.        心理学空间 d7~2D*?pE

VX&X6MC?0 Yet questions like these became increasingly difficult to ignore for Dr. Gazzaniga, as he took on a more prominent role advising policy makers on the applications of brain science. He was appointed to a Congressional technology panel in 1991; in 2002, he took a position on the President’s Council on Bioethics. And in 2007, he became the founding director of the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation’s Law and Neuroscience Project, which tracks and evaluates applications in the legal system.        心理学空间x9^Av&k IZ'r+Z

$iogp2h"@5wYn0 There, in particular, brain science has had a growing impact. In recent years lawyers have begun to present brain images as evidence, usually to mitigate responsibility for a crime or to test veracity of testimony, as in a polygraph; increasingly, those images have been admitted. And more are coming: In imaging studies, for instance, neuroscientists have identified cortical areas that are highly active when people suppress impulses or other behaviors.        

0f;OMm \\-R!i0

Eo S/AF7UQN0 But there are clear shortcomings in the application of each of these methods in courtrooms. Brain images are snapshots, for one thing; they capture a brain state at only one moment in time and say nothing about its function before or after. For another, the images vary widely among people with healthy brains — that is, a “high” level of activity in one person may be normal in another. Can brain science tell exactly where automatic processes end and self-directed “responsible” ones end?        

Y e!aQW4P5]1oZ$RR0R0心理学空间/O-i9g0P*D0k9T/K

 Not now and not likely ever, Dr. Gazzaniga argues in his book. Social constructs like good judgment and free will are even further removed, and trying to define them in terms of biological processes is, in the end, a fool’s game.        心理学空间 dg] ~nT Lyl"q

心理学空间y*d W^1NV0V SgQ+?

 “My contention is that, ultimately, responsibility is a contract between two people rather than a property of the brain, and determinism has no meaning in this context,” he writes in “Who’s in Charge?”        

Alp1E%[~v:TE0

5YbhRN2U0 Like generosity and pettiness, like love and suspiciousness, responsibility is what he calls a “strongly emergent” property — a property that, though derived from biological mechanisms, is fundamentally distinct and obeys different laws, as do ice and water.        

2u} v F^M}0

L R2X!K,IU"Of0 Dr. Gazzaniga is not the first scientist making this case. It is far from a settled matter, in part because researchers do not yet have a complete picture of how automatic and deliberate systems interact biologically.        

[aZ`J4c^0心理学空间2P m^#PgGb{"c;{

 “I see Gazzaniga’s point, and it would indeed be easiest if we could ignore conclusions derived from brain science and psychology when it comes to legal issues,” said Ap Dijksterhuis, a psychologist at Radboud University Nijmegen, in the Netherlands, in an e-mail. “However, I do not think we can do this forever, and at some point, some key legal concepts such as accountability or responsibility will have to be redefined.”        

+S$Vr4Hsn$c5_&[0

:]_hw9T'o(b0 Until then, Dr. Gazzaniga’s advice is to look for them where they’ve always been: in the hearts and moral intuitions of human beings, in their laws and customs.        心理学空间1Fl"_&d9Tt&W'sR

心理学空间.UAD(A DsMT

 And, it should be said, in their stories.        心理学空间$Wf F'G4K

心理学空间 p)d z#HX8{U7AuF

This article has been revised to reflect the following correction:心理学空间1tm%I Fa0bNE

心理学空间7cF#r1JY ^zS,J

Correction: November 3, 2011

ASz~C(n _i0

os@2b-^-?&K0Because of an editing error, an article on Tuesday about the neuroscientist Michael S. Gazzaniga misidentified a program at the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation that had Dr. Gazzaniga as its founding director. It is the Law and Neuroscience Project, not the Research Network on Law and Neuroscience. (The research network, whose founding director is Owen D. Jones, is a more recent program of the foundation.)心理学空间 SFA"R1kV dW,A

 www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

Michael S. GazzanigaMichael S. Gazzaniga

TAG: Gazzaniga 认知神经科学
«没有了 Michael Gazzaniga加扎尼加
《Michael Gazzaniga加扎尼加》
认知神经科学之父——迈克尔·葛詹尼加»