THE ORIGINS OF ATTACHMENT THEORY: JOHN BOWLBY AND MARY AINSWORTH
作者: INGE BRETHERTON / 36273次阅读 时间: 2011年4月24日
来源: Developmental Psychology (1992), 28, 759-775.
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

_ xB*h9s0REFINING ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH: 心理学空间 C\-i(t#^|
心理学空间7` L ~.x0~%L X
BOWLBY AND AINSWORTH
*k6B;FCpQx0
z.uW1l&q"e ch N0Before the publication of “The Nature of the Child’s Tie to His Mother” in 1958, Mary 心理学空间b/b Bxd
Ainsworth received a preprint of the paper from John Bowlby. This event led Bowlby and
9u'c5G"Mt+?bj0Ainsworth to renew their close intellectual collaboration. Ainsworth’s subsequent analysis of data
U6|K!x0C A'^,a0from her Ganda project (Ainsworth 1963, 1967) influenced and was influenced by Bowlby’s
A~1n2],cF0reformulation of attachment theory (published in 1969). In this sharing of ideas, Ainsworth’s
!e8\HgH'mcX x^0theoretical contribution to Bowlby’s presentation of the ontogeny of human attachment cannot be 心理学空间@B;O9fl(z
overestimated. 心理学空间(JUaLl)a ^W

CS3vNp9z.l d\0Findings From Ainsworth’s Ganda Project
;N#R5_nvQ0a;?o0
eu Qx c H0a_p0The Ganda data (Ainsworth, 1963, 1967) were a rich source for the study of individual 心理学空间z Z;P3c \.r'f
differences in the quality of mother - infant interaction, the topic that Bowlby had earlier left aside
V.G-X)?)Z?0as too difficult to study. Of special note, in light of Ainsworth’s future work, was an evaluation of 心理学空间`4{zw[3I!F
maternal sensitivity to infant signals, derived from interview data. Mothers who were excellent
_%Ml"U.K4U+D0informants and who provided much spontaneous detail were rated as highly sensitive, in contrast
A5B2K{Xd4Mw0to other mothers who seemed imperceptive of the nuances of infant behavior. Three infant
5p+w6o1?&N+^0
p+dzpp#TW'T'`l$v@0心理学空间/xah,z[8v
attachment patterns were observed: Securely attached infants cried little and seemed content to 心理学空间}Dr$]D?+Y5Z].E
explore in the presence of mother; insecurely attached infants cried frequently, even when held by 心理学空间/C#N4OGy-AV;X:M
their mothers, and explored little; and not-yet attached infants manifested no differential behavior 心理学空间"J%q!C@D4D
to the mother. 心理学空间9@%bA/FjSmv
心理学空间|N]'~:F)GV
It turned out that secure attachment was significantly correlated with maternal sensitivity.
!Z*f haS&jQ:gk0Babies of sensitive mothers tended to be securely attached, whereas babies of less sensitive
5Vbyg7uA Zk0mothers were more likely to he classified as insecure. Mothers’ enjoyment of breast-feeding also 心理学空间&k7o;j1J8^SoqDf R
correlated with infant security. These findings foreshadow some of Ainsworth’s later work,
0?3y!e%rS%l0although the measures are not yet as sophisticated as those developed for subsequent studies. 心理学空间4cn_tSlnW

&QFc1M*D0Ainsworth presented her initial findings from the Ganda project at meetings of the Tavistock
eq-} o ~'I/Y6}0Study Group organized by Bowlby during the 1960s (Ainsworth, 1963). Participants invited to
Y*uW e@wL,U0these influential gatherings included many now-eminent infant researchers of diverse theoretical
kgDm4_z&s0backgrounds (in addition to Mary Ainsworth, there were Genevieve Appell, Miriam David, Jacob
6faI1L p0Gewirtz, Hanus Papousek, Heinz Prechtl, Harriet Rheingold, Henry Ricciuti, Louis Sander, and 心理学空间3BW#|^a
Peter Wolff), as well as renowned animal researchers such as Harry Harlow, Robert Hinde,
4VS$GA$~\"NG0Charles Kaufmann, Jay Rosenblatt, and Thelma Rowell Their lively discussions and ensuing
nAcS W H3s+B8lRn0studies contributed much to the developing field of infant social development in general. 心理学空间 {g7Y$mP3FF R g
Importantly for Bowlby, they also enriched his ongoing elaboration of attachment theory. Bowlby 心理学空间3Vc$Lt$c|Y]
had always believed that he had much to gain from bringing together researchers with different
b%?U r4m$d!Pk0theoretical backgrounds (e.g., learning theory, psychoanalysis, and ethology), whether or not thy 心理学空间 }-_7x AT9hk(p2F
agreed with his theoretical position. Proceedings of these fruitful meetings were published in four
;Nh5GU#YYY I0volumes entitled Determinants of Infant Behaviour (1961, 1963, 1965, and 1969, edited by Brian
_]%Fh6@2v0Foss).
-E6o.w-Tx)uv0
}O P9Z)MO0The Baltimore Project
#{ G)v_C'h+v0
2P&hl'j&j5j0In 1963, while still pondering the data from the Ganda study, Mary Ainsworth embarked on
(WLrl6\ DU#@0a second observational project whose thoroughness no researcher has since equaled. Again, she
$]| Z+R6J#a+L4O0opted for naturalistic observations, hut with interviews playing a somewhat lesser role. The 26 心理学空间/s"|6b$oMZsV'e1Z
participating Baltimore families were recruited before their babies were horn, with 18 home visits 心理学空间J)`\7t6Sc
beginning in the baby’s first month and ending at 54 weeks of age. Each visit lasted 4 hours to
M1EQ#Hsl"Lu]C#M+j0make sure that mothers would feel comfortable enough to follow their normal routine, resulting
c_9KqT4EPq%})q ^0in approximately 72 hours of data collection per family. 心理学空间.`sG-M.YN}]

C(`y'L@R0Raw data took the form of narrative reports, jotted down in personal shorthand, marked in 心理学空间 nkq1F+A
5-minute intervals, and later dictated into a tape recorder for transcription. Typed narratives from 心理学空间W.h5o Fcet!F
all visits for each quarter of the first year of life were grouped together for purposes of analysis. 心理学空间{GEbv4}/z2k P

,U#Y[*O&cRb-EN0A unique (at the time) aspect of Ainsworth’s methodology was the emphasis on meaningful 心理学空间#qy3@Pirt I;~
behavioral patterns in context, rather than on frequency counts of specific behaviors, This
O"TM)bB+JC}M8v0approach had roots in her dissertation work, in which she classified patterns of familial and 心理学空间8Pb;P[:X
extrafamilial dependent and independent security, in her expertise with the Rorschach test, and in
*s5A(jk+OL$^0her work at the Tavistock Institute with Bowlby and Robertson. 心理学空间!n.CNc3R$sY
心理学空间 } F~7Wh
Close examination of the narratives revealed the emergence of characteristic mother-infant
6|O[Q8z1QR0interaction patterns during the first 3 months (see Ainsworth et al., 1978; see also Ainsworth,
Bj{ SG01982, 1983). Separate analyses were conducted on feeding situations (Ainsworth & Bell, 1969),
5jW:Wg+T0mother-infant face-to-face interaction (Blehar, Lieberman, & Ainsworth, 1977), crying (Bell &
Dt(CKL*Q |q0Ainsworth, 1972), infant greeting and following (Stayton & Ainsworth, 1973), the attachment-心理学空间 V-Dm+n$C%u7N
exploration balance (Ainsworth, Bell, & Stayton, 1971), obedience (Stayton, Hogan, & 心理学空间 jC+nqyK
Ainsworth, 1973), close bodily contact (Ainsworth, Bell, Blehar, & Main, 1971), approach 心理学空间 brcPKE w(Z&f
behavior (Tracy, Lamb, & Ainsworth, 1976), and affectionate contact (Tracy & Ainsworth, 心理学空间 AT+R+a2v4zW/q9S
1981). 心理学空间A7W/JH;Y f:?:S l
心理学空间B:RO$Xaf3^
Striking individual differences were observed in how sensitively, appropriately, and 心理学空间1Juq u-g&bs
promptly mothers responded to their infants’ signals. For some mother-infant pairs, feeding was 心理学空间"c'd%|Yn6i2}3`
an occasion for smooth cooperation. Other mothers had difficulties in adjusting their pacing and
CRBB {:x$o@P$V$W0behavior to the baby’s cues. In response, their babies tended to struggle, choke, and spit up,
ND|:]4Mg6Fz&b0hardly the sensuous oral experience Freud had had in mind. Similar distinctive patterns were
5V3g+N#@ot|g^G_0observed in face-to-face interactions between mother and infant during the period from 6 to 15 心理学空间9Hd-M uCm R
weeks (Blehar et al,, 1977). When mothers meshed their own playful behavior with that of their 心理学空间)De%_L|0M#W.G
babies, infants responded with joyful bouncing, smiling, and vocalizing. However, when mothers 心理学空间(_]l|-m]+M
initiated face-to-face interactions silently and with an unsmiling expression, ensuing interactions
?N#jSU^1k0were muted and brief. Findings on close bodily contact resembled those on feeding and
M-ab&j)Q8fD6Nv0face-to-face Interaction, as did those on crying. There were enormous variations in how many 心理学空间E4b*Qp_:}6@2n+P!t$xv
crying episodes a mother ignored and how long she let the baby cry. In countering those who
Xy.TJS ?${yQ0M3oD0argued that maternal responsiveness might lead to “spoiling,” Bell and Ainsworth (1972) 心理学空间P+u9T Wq[
concluded that “an infant whose mother’s responsiveness helps him to achieve his ends develops 心理学空间})@E'`d
confidence in his own ability to control what happens to him” (p. 1188). 心理学空间TRM+i _B5{p [$@

9a"l0J9]6O8A(Q]!C0Maternal sensitivity in the first quarter was associated with more harmonious mother-infant 心理学空间R*yDW;K$M"Dn
relationships In the fourth quarter. Babies whose mothers had been highly responsive to crying 心理学空间N)`H2EQCmd
during the early months now tended to cry less, relying for communication on facial expressions,
N[E3\&L9RJ N0gestures, and vocalizations (Bell & Ainsworth, 1972). Similarly, infants whose mothers had 心理学空间]3s$jJ1H7l}o
provided much tender holding during the first quarter sought contact less often during the fourth
;{5Jw z]^_'u0quarter, hut when contact occurred, it was rated as more satisfying and affectionate (Ainsworth, 心理学空间2l }O| uZT `$FlU
Bell, Blehar, et al,, 1971), Ainsworth (Ainsworth et al., 1978) explains these findings by recourse 心理学空间R~)w.x"]G~J
to infants’ expectations, based on prior satisfying or rejecting experiences with mother.
]P9fM*l:Jks0
-w6[ gIawA$~,Q0All first-quarter interactive patterns were also related to infant behavior in a laboratory procedure
T*N X ?3u8G,G0known as the Strange Situation (Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969). This initially very controversial 心理学空间.c"Z_ Q.V'{ |8N
laboratory procedure for 1 -year-olds was originally designed to examine the balance of attachment 心理学空间e^`oL'd"}n
and exploratory behaviors under conditions of low and high stress, a topic in which
!Yli _~8],^}U0Harlow (196!) had aroused Ainsworth’s interest during meetings of the Tavistock group, but 心理学空间U;]y{w,J,Q)@p
which also reminded her of an earlier study by Arsenian (1943) on young children in an insecure 心理学空间T_ |6]J)i\ {
situation and of her dissertation work on security theory. 心理学空间+_f9o$@Oh"D
心理学空间n)q"tw __
The Strange Situation is a 20-minute miniature drama with eight episodes. Mother and 心理学空间m/[8PR Q
infant are introduced to a laboratory playroom, where they are later joined by an unfamiliar
aJ5j4TZZ9NT0woman. While the stranger plays with the baby, the mother leaves briefly and then returns. A 心理学空间 clx:b9df
second separation ensues during which the baby is completely alone. Finally, the stranger and then 心理学空间"f$C+o;t`7m+fBb;{X
the mother return. 心理学空间HT:s7qw/H~

$m)vjpW$XL.QA p0As expected, Ainsworth found that infants explored the playroom and toys more vigorously
o9P:fln#R(B {!X2w0in the presence of their mothers than after a stranger entered or while the mother was absent 心理学空间 @ RZe zy&{3s6?
(Ainsworth & Bell, 1970). Although these results were theoretically interesting, Ainsworth
-WD/\:z2J,g\B+G|0became much more intrigued with unexpected patterns of infant reunion behaviors, which
0Zo;} j/k N6X zt0reminded her of responses Robertson had documented in children exposed to prolonged 心理学空间j6Tu+C*v.|0~!w@S
separations, and about which Bowlby (1959) had theorized in his paper on separation.
:CYO2M#Z:G"I}$UV0心理学空间c[)a L)Z,cw
A few of the I -year-olds from the Baltimore study were surprisingly angry when the mother 心理学空间D:D `Y$w7`k,V6O;D3[
returned after a 3-minute (or shorter) separation. They cried and wanted contact but would not 心理学空间)]G[-Gq
simply cuddle or “sink in” when picked up by the returning mother. Instead, they showed their 心理学空间'bP5MF7hg#K
ambivalence by kicking or swiping at her. Another group of children seemed to snub or avoid the
0Pz@"|3_;{)T0mother on reunion, even though they had often searched for her while she was gone. Analyses of 心理学空间5f g9skmGJ!ta
home data revealed that those infants who had been ambivalent toward or avoidant of the mother 心理学空间E#Jq S?'`*Dx-C
on reunion in the Strange Situation had a less harmonious relationship with her at home than 心理学空间{*}0nX)y!z`3kU
those (a majority) who sought proximity, interaction, or contact on reunion (Ainsworth, Bell, &
7te!N1z+y5H|0Stayton, 1974). Thus originated the well-known Strange Situation classification system
Y$}qe1f0(Ainsworth et al., 1978), which, to Ainsworth’s chagrin, has stolen the limelight from her
8\V-NBL2lY0observational findings of naturalistic mother-infant interaction patterns at home.
Noq$X1\7N0
_^$]7ak|t0The First Volume in the Attachment Trilogy: Attachment and Ethology 心理学空间Dx&v*Oo4vxv

io6Z ?N1dHBj9Q0While Ainsworth wrote up the findings from her Ganda study for Infancy in Uganda (1967) and
4N9^Z*u ab0was engaged in collecting data for the Baltimore project, Bowlby worked on the first volume of
st1Z8Gzz#k0the attachment trilogy, Attachment (1969). When he began this enterprise in 1962, the plan had
3^"CIYo s$v)P} ["@0been for a single hook. However, as he explains in the preface: “As my study of theory progressed 心理学空间g_ ~0IY
it was gradually borne in upon me that the field I had set out to plough so light-heartedly
o)w-iEE0was no less than the one Freud had started tilling sixty years earlier.” In short, Bowlby realized 心理学空间cI{(KO$Io"S
that he had to develop a new theory of motivation and behavior control, built on up-to-date science
4|F+OxW/b-W#hk0rather than the outdated psychic energy model espoused by Freud. 心理学空间qyf/k)odI

B8v8ui |GZ n*Nt0In the first half of Attachment, Bowlby lays the groundwork for such a theory, taking pains
3[ xo8?rb\R;D0to document each important statement with available research findings. He begins by noting that 心理学空间EZ|5W3fp.mu6U
organisms at different levels of the phylogenetic scale regulate instinctive behavior in distinct 心理学空间!P`-G/F q x
ways, ranging from primitive reflex-like “fixed action patterns” to complex plan hierarchies with
P'ly V y:\$j+g-J$wl0subgoals. In the most complex organisms, instinctive behaviors may be “goal-corrected” with
\9w0f:F5IN^0continual on-course adjustments (such as a bird of prey adjusting its flight to the movements of 心理学空间UnQ2t8S%v0LX)a
the prey). The concept of cybernetically controlled behavioral systems organized as plan
d;lB5[.lNS6Ht0
#Z/[,S |q)c)k0
Uy$T,zm+F,b4Fv{$q0hierarchies (Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960) thus came to replace Freud’s concept of drive and
r{7f/w wy-Y0instinct. Behaviors regulated by such systems need not be rigidly innate, hut-depending on the
+b2VRl(U%s3z0organism- can adapt in greater or lesser degrees to changes in environmental circumstances, 心理学空间+_z'D(eT n+D;P
provided that these do not deviate too much from the organism’s environment of evolutionary 心理学空间t!^O9ta*h2G
adaptedness. Such flexible organisms pay a price, however, because adaptable behavioral systems 心理学空间*?P0lt h3?^
can more easily be subverted from their optimal path of development. For humans, Bowlby 心理学空间i'qsSQzq
speculates, the environment of evolutionary adaptedness probably resembles that of present-day 心理学空间];f;Q @7~ j#~2i3R
hunter-gatherer societies.
9K0B%l2V#u z^B0心理学空间8yF#B \1C&V:Cd
The ultimate functions of behavioral systems controlling attachment, parenting, mating, 心理学空间2fi&L-E,C;Z4|
feeding, and exploration are survival and procreation. In some cases, the predictable outcome of 心理学空间(x"T ^.dh#W
system activation is a time-limited behavior (such as food intake); in others it is the time-extended
+y9^'W @ R}*l c0maintenance of an organism in a particular relation to its environment (e.g., within its own
2UK}(Pc9d)B.x"uo2k0territory or in proximity to particular companions).
hx4}:G9fW)L?2|0心理学空间Os9G i W1u(Le
Complex behavioral systems of the kind proposed by Bowlby can work with foresight in organisms 心理学空间^B HZ$y0LkA*u-F
that have evolved an ability to construct internal working models of the environment and of 心理学空间1k{}p&D8xsd!B f,K
their own actions in it (a concept taken over from (Craik, 1943, through the writings of the biologist
X0Ke&`0iU(y/m X-pJ0J. Young, 1964). The more adequate an organism’s internal working model, the more accurately
`)f&t*hQ!?;m0the organism can predict the future, However, adds Bowlby, if working models of the
'G4a%i$v"y ]&VRy0environment and self are out of date or are only half revised after drastic environmental change, 心理学空间6wcjy'f`;l }V K-i
pathological functioning may ensue. He speculates that useful model revision, extension, and 心理学空间z tbM:JMB
consistency checking may require conscious processing of model content. In humans, communicative 心理学空间o$E4Lfg0pI
processes-initially limited to emotional or gestural signaling and later including language 心理学空间| e:LJ,fGt%b
心理学空间(W+\ S&[]
-also permit the inter-subjective sharing of model content. On an intrapsychic level, the same 心理学空间%Kz![Z1]z6Bd~2~0?c
processes are useful for self-regulation and behavioral priority setting.
}BUT[0In mammals and birds, behavioral systems tend to become organized during specific 心理学空间rw'r4Lf&e'k A
sensitive developmental periods. As initial reflex-like behavior chains come under more complex, 心理学空间#}$F/hG'\1_I
cybernetically controlled organization, the range of stimuli that can activate them also becomes
*M%a"k E#tq#m0more restricted, This is the case in imprinting, broadly defined as the restriction of specific 心理学空间:bJ p7D e
instinctive behaviors to particular individuals or groups of individuals during sensitive phases of 心理学空间%jRWE*H |f
心理学空间Qo$d};O8n

:E+Zm9z!ig8L8W$P0development, as in filial, parental, and sexual imprinting.
&?c cAe*IU0心理学空间&B|$QzY6Dg c
Having laid out this general theory of motivation and behavior regulation in the first half of 心理学空间 a@,~I!Bo
the volume, Bowlby goes on, in the second half, to apply these ideas to the specific domain of 心理学空间:A@#i!dZl
infant-mother attachment. He defines attachment behavior as behavior that has proximity to an
-naDk~}~0attachment figure as a predictable outcome and whose evolutionary function is protection of the
#Pg+U Pn7X|0infant from danger, insisting that attachment has its own motivation and is in no way derived from
m HBdkp/f+R0systems subserving mating and feeding.
HER8o XU0心理学空间6K,ux)O,K7{'E
Although human infants initially direct proximity-promoting signals fairly indiscriminately to 心理学空间!T!m4EQ'Yg8syhQ
all caregivers, these behaviors become increasingly focused on those primary figures who are 心理学空间S1d2X1z Ks4fSlP
responsive to the infant’s crying and who engage the infant in social interaction (Schaffer & 心理学空间3y~C]:R"S
Emerson, 1964). Once attached, locomotor infants are able to use the attachment figure as a
EJ[F|u3xI0secure base for exploration of the environment and as a safe haven to which to return for
1H+v}:K(}I[4n^7\1R y0reassurance (Ainsworth, 1967; Schaffer & Emerson, 1964). How effectively the attachment 心理学空间!P Ks8\zi8G7Q m
figure can serve in these roles depends on the quality of social interaction-especially the 心理学空间9vBa-so
attachment figure’s sensitivity to the infant’s signals-although child factors also play a role. 心理学空间 X N/gQ b9Y9y'E
Building on Ainsworth’s Ganda study (1967) and preliminary findings from her Baltimore 心理学空间8l onMV+{)` r.I
project, Bowlby (1969) comments that: 心理学空间p2Pg2z9H7{

!c8U/iDVl"p6^P4|+J0when interaction between a couple runs smoothly, each party manifests intense pleasure in 心理学空间:NM wL5x~e)K_0q
the other’s company and especially in the other’s expression of affection. Conversely,
q|+L+f+j2{0whenever interaction results in persistent conflict each party is likely on occasion to exhibit 心理学空间sCh)lt\h(~
intense anxiety or unhappiness, especially when the other is rejecting. Proximity and 心理学空间s'Z+BtJI:?3l$F
affectionate interchange are appraised and felt as pleasurable by both, whereas distance and
e9k5E'Tk6eV0expressions of rejection are appraised as disagreeable or painful by both. (p. 242) 心理学空间]4S#e c(W2K3Z*D Mu"u S

C l c6N ?2lg0During the preschool years, the attachment behavioral system, always complementary to 心理学空间Q Bm5u+chP`3Z
the parental caregiving system, undergoes further reorganization as the child attains growing insight 心理学空间2K3o cY1U }/~+h:G
into the attachment figure’s motives and plans. Bowlby refers to this stage as goal-心理学空间 ~&`uEi!k ?
corrected partnership. However, in emphasizing infant initiative and sensitive maternal responding,
zx!X?)QDE4E0D0Bowlby’s (1951) earlier theorizing on the mother as the child’s ego and superego was regrettably 心理学空间,d JSL%B6v
lost. 心理学空间3B2c2o @[eA!Q7tz
心理学空间~FW5l/hx D
Consolidation 心理学空间#^1f+fci$b(B
心理学空间9b4P7escd)V_3W

2z,sd%^H:j0The publication of the first volume of the attachment trilogy in 1969 coincided with the
O_-b |PKpvlC0appearance in print of initial findings from Ainsworth’s Baltimore project (reviewed earlier).
l9m8U"_+y f4}0However, many investigators strongly contested Ainsworth’s claims regarding the meaning of
m.v3C D,OH+o/}{0Strange Situation behavior, often because they failed to note that Strange Situation classifications
I^q9z.S[?2y0had been validated against extensive home observations. Some interpreted avoidant infants’
D,_({,n ?D8{0behavior as independence. The controversy lessened somewhat after the publication of Patterns 心理学空间`:qJFS
of Attachment (Ainsworth et al., 1978), which drew together the results from the Baltimore
M4r hx6L&lP0project and presented findings from other laboratories on the sequelae of attachment classifications 心理学空间 VGy9]d
in toddlerhood and early childhood (e.g., Main, 1973; Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 1978). 心理学空间*epG jh i?Q)}

)S q3APw7X"V'xm l0During this period, many of Ainsworth’s graduate students began to publish their own work.
;DYuv$O2S5S0Silvia Bell (1970) examined the relationship between object permanence and attachment. Mary
L,i&_8nI)K3n0Main (1973) studied secure and insecure toddlers’ capacity to become invested in play activities 心理学空间t/d6E5NCr2kp
and problem solving. Mary Blehar (1974) undertook the first study of attachment and nonmaternal
!L$o s6Rc}O#u0care, and Alicia Lieberman (1977) investigated attachment and peer relationships in 心理学空间*n_&}X9wu;k
preschoolers. Mary Ainsworth’s influence is also evident in the fact that many Johns Hopkins 心理学空间#\I!}Iv r#xw&u
undergraduate students who had helped with the analysis of data from the Baltimore project later 心理学空间U{e,OSZ
produced innovative dissertations on attachment-related topics at their respective graduate
x'Q-BqI*_[0institutions. Among these students were Robert Marvin (1972, 1977), who wrote on the 心理学空间4];D#vBT2t
goal-corrected partnership; Milton Kotelchuck (1972), who studied father attachment; Mark
LT`qO&BaR8fU0Cummings (1980), who investigated attachment and day care; Mark Greenberg (Greenberg &
O"em1b[9l,|"j%A0Marvin, 1979), who examined attachment in deaf children; and Everett Waters (1978), who
g?vL^'Za FX5c [0documented the longitudinal stability of attachment patterns from 12 to 18 months.
0N;?*t6eLcwz%eC0
$_1c4A vbbuW0Qs0Everett Waters’ entry into graduate study at the University of Minnesota in 1973 had a 心理学空间"T~aR$K2U!w?b m
profound effect on Alan Sroufe, who had read Mary Ainsworth’s (1968) theoretical article about 心理学空间:Gg}D*\$HJ1{M1]r
object relations and dependency but had not heard of the Strange Situation or the Baltimore project
G'w!z TuL0(Sroufe, personal communication, 1988). Sroufe’s contact with Waters led to significant empirical 心理学空间EJw/~4}#r"i6d T xD#Z
and theoretical collaborations. In 1977, Sroufe and Waters wrote an influential paper that
%\+F$WkxVC0made attachment as an organizational construct accessible to a large audience. At the same time, 心理学空间|S(X.O-QB3x5c"B)vR
Sroufe and Egeland, together with many of their students, undertook a large-scale longitudinal 心理学空间aEy9_$}o&}
心理学空间.i)\\ J+J%l*GlZ

f#~I7I?E0wf|0study of attachment with an at-risk population (disadvantaged mothers), The Minnesota study, 心理学空间!^v!M%w/I)p
summarized in Sroufe (1983) but still ongoing, stands as the second major longitudinal study of
(h-n{q'~X-F0the relationship between quality of caregiving and security of attachment. 心理学空间E JDEm znV

3F1l-G(b Cs(Z0Elsewhere across the United States, much time was spent testing the predictive validity of
} G'`2^}!Q2t%p0Strange Situation reunion classifications. Many researchers sought to train with Mary Ainsworth
op2S _!S/uRu8v0or her former students to learn the procedure and classification system. Hundreds of studies using
CM'f.X9p*Z'V0the Strange Situation appeared in print. It often seemed as if attachment and the Strange Situation
`v _@*q^6]_0had become synonymous.
-W&[^)Uz&k!a5c;v]1J0心理学空间f8nt}-b!N z!_

www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

«玛丽·爱因斯沃斯的陌生情境测验 安思沃斯 Ainsworth
《安思沃斯 Ainsworth》
没有了»