THE ORIGINS OF ATTACHMENT THEORY: JOHN BOWLBY AND MARY AINSWORTH
作者: INGE BRETHERTON / 36275次阅读 时间: 2011年4月24日
来源: Developmental Psychology (1992), 28, 759-775.
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网心理学空间.{,vzg};u&U

THE FORMULATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY AND THE 心理学空间V^bKpH
心理学空间XtW3F3MpgW
FIRST ATTACHMENT STUDY 心理学空间'O^o(O\*rE.{2L

1`b p]z0Theoretical Formulations 心理学空间(\6xy"}y
心理学空间O v i/s8m%h7NlrK
Bowlby’s first formal statement of attachment theory, building on concepts from ethology 心理学空间Qy6C1Kzj7c t%p
and developmental psychology, was presented to the British Psychoanalytic Society in London in 心理学空间%i%vO y+]8N
three now classic papers: “The Nature of the Child’s Tie to His Mother” (1958), “Separation
!C0Am |fg0Anxiety” (1959), and “Grief and Mourning in Infancy and Early Childhood” (1960). By 1962
6dp(vB8]Y0Bowlby had completed two further papers (never published; 1962 a and b) on defensive processes
F~4wRt ~CE0related to mourning. These five papers represent the first basic blueprint of attachment theory. 心理学空间*p$gI5e.i_\
心理学空间C7?6R3b/XTv S
The Nature of the Child’s Tie to His Mother
i5[M2V.Bw6D0
-k{I2]y5MC0This paper reviews and then rejects those contemporary psychoanalytic explanations for the
Q0ps!? M0child’s libidinal tie to the mother in which need satisfaction is seen as primary and attachment as 心理学空间0|1}u!n"q'bE
secondary or derived. Borrowing from Freud’s (1905/1953) notion that mature human sexuality
dHX j)r/k.vCk0is built up of component instincts, Bowlby proposed that I 2-month-olds’ unmistakable attach心理学空间U*BF)o9T*]2Nb4x
ment behavior is made up of a number of component instinctual responses that have the function 心理学空间 \GyU\
of binding the infant to the mother and the mother to the infant. These component responses 心理学空间 g+Fwh%\`'t'g]
(among them sucking, clinging, and following, as well as the signaling behaviors of smiling and 心理学空间2z)q jo[ PE0\:?:q
crying) mature relatively independently during the first year of life and become increasingly 心理学空间*_ @/B h$r
integrated and focused on a mother figure during the second 6 months. Bowlby saw clinging and 心理学空间zJ(pZ;i4G ~2m,e v
following as possibly more important for attachment than sucking and crying.
;zd"ti~7?8N-@B0心理学空间ua-[;i"L Q T
To buttress his arguments, Bowlby reviewed data from existing empirical studies of infants’
S]x?)C6f+U8b0cognitive and social development, including those of Piaget (1951, 1954), with whose ideas he 心理学空间 ? X0nwb'OB
had become acquainted during a series of meetings by the ‘Psychobiology of the Child” study
:z%oI7a2Fe"rD.|0group, organized by the same Ronald I Hargreaves at the World Health Organization who had 心理学空间$S-w%~)dOE q\ @
commissioned Bowlby’s 1951 report. These informative meetings, also attended by Erik Erikson, 心理学空间#M ?~4iS*W9yY yy
Julian Huxley, Baerbel Inhelder, Konrad Lorenz, Margaret Mead, and Ludwig von Bertalanffy,
4Jm&q:rU;]Pp;t.a0took place between 1953 and 1956. (Proceedings were published by Tavistock Publications.) For 心理学空间x1w8X9\6D`
additional evidence, Bowlby drew on many years of experience as weekly facilitator of a support 心理学空间(^%}m#\Q JH!laY-F/P
group for young mothers in London.
t7p%\[YB0
7}N,o+WCP#t0After his careful discussion of infant development, Bowlby introduced ethological concepts,
d @ c1w3ZV^0such as sign stimuli or social releasers that “cause” specific responses to he activated and shut off 心理学空间1pTF!aT3D
or terminated (see Tinbergen, 1951). These stimuli could he external or intrapsychic, an important 心理学空间a/gn E ZT]8a
point in view of the fact that some psychoanalysts accused Bowlby of behaviorism because he
c*WPd Zl!a0supposedly ignored mental phenomena. Bowlby also took great pains to draw a clear distinction
[S"c d ekdL0between the old social learning theory concept of dependency and the new concept of attachment, 心理学空间^T9h!bW vtH Q$j
noting that attachment is not indicative of regression, hut rather performs a natural, healthy 心理学空间8z/xDHw!ez
function even in adult life.
j.LM#X Au0
sV K~_Pd0Bowlby’s new instinct theory raised quite a storm at the British Psychoanalytic Society. 心理学空间|%_} R9KP f0k
Even Bowlby’s own analyst, Joan Riviere, protested. Anna Freud, who missed the meeting but 心理学空间0^[P4e;h
read the paper, politely wrote:
%MBK5AR5K1HI3d8`,S0心理学空间ti7l@xkaO
“Dr. Bowlby is too valuable a person to get lost to psychoanalysis” (Grosskurth, 1987).
'K!g3Hm4EQ7d0心理学空间JSQo w,@+t
Separation Anxiety 心理学空间ls y%^1||,M c

#i#j^ IF+Jq0心理学空间9jRY K i ~D0{
The second seminal paper (Bowlby, 1959) builds on observations by Robertson (1953b) and
,knEb:V8`9s0Heinicke (1956; later elaborated as Heinicke & Westheimer, 1966), as well as on Harlow and
`z gY)N Kf?0Zimmermann’s (1958) groundbreaking work on the effects of maternal deprivation in rhesus
y c^(r8v"[g(O0monkeys. Traditional theory, Bowlby claims, can explain neither the intense attachment of infants
1|6f%\:^7V;X#P^-Z_ c0and young children to a mother figure nor their dramatic responses to separation. 心理学空间N\4R+y [UeZ?t
心理学空间"z3JTDz#r|
Robertson (Robertson & Bowlby, 1952) had identified three phases of separation response: 心理学空间&l)HY:tD(Y5P:e
protest (related to separation anxiety), despair (related to grief and mourning), and denial or
G(N*c"g5wP3a {N,W/cf0detachment (related to defence mechanisms, especially repression). Again drawing on ethological 心理学空间vS2`N!E"D f.l%P
concepts regarding the control of behavior, Bowlby maintained that infants and children experience
Od@{3C/]0_*~+T(@0separation anxiety when a situation activates both escape and attachment behavior hut an 心理学空间l]2F x"F5Zm3e
attachment figure is not available. 心理学空间U!|'V;O*Or.v

}+[JU+~4|f$t0The following quote explains, in part, why some psychoanalytic colleagues called Bowlby a
3b` SlB/Xdi7\J0behaviorist: “for to have a deep attachment for a person (or a place or thing) is to have taken
BIG.dq3U\2| il0them as the terminating object of our instinctual responses” (Bowlby, 1959, p. 13). The oddity of
8b(l5B+P%x4mA"i0this statement derives from mixing, in the same sentence, experiential language (to have a deep 心理学空间Y9@ N,H:s'i M2o
attachment) with explanatory language representing an external observer’s point of view (the
*@B%U,ttF O.D)s2W&S7\0attachment figure as the terminating object). 心理学空间 sfelZEa

Qw;w?9Xv"X0In this paper, Bowlby also took issue with Freud’s claim that maternal overgratification is a
J s^!@ JX0danger in infancy. Freud failed to realize, says Bowlby, that maternal pseudo-affection and 心理学空间:Mj1[7pB8X
overprotection may derive from a mother’s overcompensation for unconscious hostility. In 心理学空间3@,~8i"^:hO a4B'X
Bowlby’s view, excessive separation anxiety is due to adverse family experiences-such as 心理学空间1S7f ["G1mb
repeated threats of abandonment or rejection by parents-or to a parent’s or sibling’s illness or
|2ni-y$u+t0death for which the child feels responsible. 心理学空间 l!X)u s{3|s

,dVT"TRsK0Bowlby also pointed out that, in some cases, separation anxiety can be excessively low or be
` i*t:uo(b0altogether absent, giving an erroneous impression of maturity. He attributes pseudo-心理学空间.G*A,^;I[#N
independence under these conditions to defensive processes. A well-loved child, he claims, is
Z H.S)H#od0G!~0quite likely to protest separation from parents but will later develop more self-reliance, These 心理学空间T+ui:ZW[mW
ideas reemerged later in Ainsworth’s classifications of ambivalent, avoidant, and secure patterns 心理学空间EgI|(m
of infant-mother attachment (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). 心理学空间6Sr4s~%E-^ccy8Y|

/E^|/fd,q0
?@X z%Ne;~'Z5[0Grief and Mourning in Infancy and Early Childhood
6b]9D4n5sNm.T o[0心理学空间 fk)EbE!I\Q%G
In the third, most controversial paper, Bowlby (1960) questioned Anna Freud’s contention
`&BV;Fo0pb4[&`$fh0that bereaved infants cannot mourn because of insufficient ego development and therefore
kc O$Vm)~5~,r0experience nothing more than brief bouts of separation anxiety if an adequate substitute caregiver 心理学空间q_#pw}
is available. In contrast, Bowlby (citing Marris, 1958) claimed that grief and mourning processes
az!Tq f0in children and adults appear whenever attachment behaviors are activated but the attachment 心理学空间 `hK.[j
figure continues to he unavailable. He also suggested that an inability to form deep relationships 心理学空间Lv4cD pd-a+v
with others may result when the succession of substitutes is too frequent. 心理学空间,Bd,bw,Y-lEr
心理学空间8g%Yv4c;qb w2X
As with the first paper, this paper also drew strong objections from many members of the 心理学空间vO,O^e:^Jb
British Psychoanalytic Society. One analyst is said to have exclaimed: “Bowlby? Give me
+rf0Ib~M0Barrabas” (Grosskurth, 1987). Controversy also accompanied the published version of this paper
R.C7` n \1^ k!o4}C0in The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child. Unbeknownst to Bowlby, rejoinders had been invited 心理学空间 X2HM-iz]$Z#L1f
from Anna Freud (1960), Max Schur (1960), and René Spitz (1960), all of whom protested 心理学空间F @B ^6lH p.kG
various aspects of Bowlby’s revision of Freudian theory. Spitz ended his rejoinder by saying:
xH-p-M;SFU0
zRX` I0When submitting new theories we should not violate the principle of parsimony in science by
5pa _YM7D#d0offering hypotheses which in contrast to existing theory becloud the observational facts, are
Q!y#l%v;x.r ovUL0oversimplified, and make no contribution to the better understanding of observed phenomena.
:} |(` n*K#p G)ah0(p. 93) 心理学空间e e#y:S j)H
心理学空间8P;W+i2\'z2R
Despite this concerted attack, Bowlby remained a member of the British Psychoanalytic Society
0H%J+qOs3R&c0for the rest of his life, although he never again used it as a forum for discussing his ideas. At a
!w9|x:la*V?xM0meeting of the society in memory of John Bowlby, Eric Rayner (1991) expressed his regret at this 心理学空间#J:SYb+\/\
turn of events:
E'U3Q:Dr7D0心理学空间*M2e2jX3Zd e4` T
What seems wrong is when a theorist extols his own view by rubbishing others; Bowlby
M~d` {7f|0received this treatment. . . . Our therapeutic frame of mind is altered by theory. John Bowlby
Vo1]s@l0was a great alterer of frames of mind. 心理学空间+]@c)iH

z%lm"kaePA-{v'Y;b0Bowlby’s controversial paper on mourning attracted the attention of Colin Parkes, now well 心理学空间"m~"| nG6b
known for his research on adult bereavement. Parkes saw the relevance of Bowlby’s and Robertson’s
5^2]%l/t4KDJz0y!p`0work on mourning in infancy and childhood for gaining insight into the process of adult 心理学空间3@ZDX,~*X6o t0J
grief. On joining Bowlby’s research unit at the Tavistock Institute in 1962, Parkes set out to
"}vau?&rc0study a nonclinical group of widows in their homes to chart the course of nominal adult grief, 心理学空间G6Q,PZ[&x8B g
about which little was known at the time, The findings led to a joint paper with Bowlby (Bowlby
xWM1b8I0& Parkes, 1970) in which the phases of separation response delineated by Robertson for young
7V+q4XWc+^0children were elaborated into four phases of grief during adult life: (a) numbness, (h) yearning 心理学空间 v$b&FWOB] K \%O
and protest, (c) disorganization and despair, and (d) reorganization (see also Parkes, 1972).
e(PoVa0心理学空间'ga@ G2CG9Q
Before the publication of the 1970 paper, Parkes had visited Elizabeth Kubler-Ross in 心理学空间$h/[6X7p8~
Chicago, who was then gathering data for her influential book On Death and Dying (1978). The
4M%AQSDdkZX8U0phases of dying described in her book (denial, anger, bargaining, depression, and acceptance) owe 心理学空间Y%X!h1QL
much to Bowlby’s and Robertson’s thinking. Bowlby also introduced Parkes to the founder of the
u `F)C#F G7n}0modern hospice movement, Cicely Saunders. Saunders and Parkes used attachment theory and 心理学空间:{3k&Z(dH%DY
research in developing programs for the emotional care of the dying and bereaved, What they 心理学空间NG,bj l3i.V
found particularly helpful in countering negative attitudes to the dying and bereaved was the
9jLLDm0concept of grief as a process toward attaining a new identity, rather than as a state (Parkes, 心理学空间2Pg/FG.fe
personal communication, November 1989).
'VdiKg!K5M3C0心理学空间(]roz?,`X Y)Z
The First Empirical Study of Attachment: Infancy in Uganda
4K4Sq3D{A#B2v~c~0
r5GY~ q{z0Let us now return to Mary Ainsworth’s work. In late 1953, she had left the Tavistock
s)S;fa-GN.xkz5S0Clinic, obviously quite familiar with Bowlby’s thinking about ethology hut not convinced of its 心理学空间5rh%R~Aw'W
value for understanding infant- mother attachment. The Ainsworths were headed for Uganda, 心理学空间9D(X.WV7W
where Leonard Ainsworth had obtained a position at the East African Institute of Social Research
|-}E SA0at Kampala. With help from the same institute, Mary Ainsworth was able to scrape together funds 心理学空间y!@ [(m({ ky:T
for an observational study, but not before writing Bowlby a letter in which she called for empirical
#DC z7If#^3K0validation of his ethological notions (Ainsworth, January 1992, personal communication),
J^.B6\j4p o/\0心理学空间|&q+nJa0p,OJe
Inspired by her analyses of Robertson’s data, Ainsworth had initially planned an investigation
q8H)y#MD"oiV6W0of toddlers’ separation responses during weaning, but it soon became obvious that the old
q S7B{:T }b0e0ca0tradition of sending the child away “to forget the breast” had broken down. She therefore decided
m2tT'Q8Q(H+x)W0to switch gears and observe the development of infant-mother attachment. 心理学空间#EP1A \q
心理学空间zk2mZ_+Sk
As soon as she began her data collection, Ainsworth was struck by the pertinence of 心理学空间KY7]'S+O6Q {0b2@
Bowlby’s ideas, Hence, the first study of infant-mother attachment from an ethological perspective 心理学空间F'H+c|c0h W)jl}
was undertaken several years before the publication of the three seminal papers in which 心理学空间4G-`1yFZq$x2q
Bowlby (1958, 1959, 1960) laid out attachment theory. 心理学空间^a'R0[ P`c

zOLq!N1C&e[0
o[x(iQ0Ainsworth recruited 26 families with unweaned babies (ages 1 - 24 months) whom she 心理学空间qS%h8Ncmg(h'?
observed every 2 weeks for 2 hours per visit over a period of up to 9 months. Visits (with an 心理学空间Y(m GH-M3c,v5l
interpreter) took place in the family living room, where Ganda women generally entertain in the
/T SVnK&U%N0afternoon. Ainsworth was particularly interested in determining the onset of proximity-promoting
f2Y&\Jp$`0signals and behaviors, noting carefully when these signals and behaviors became preferentially
&vB3Gz5Z g7ei z0directed toward the mother. 心理学空间M3\#N0@*G2s
心理学空间L tB7a,ASB ry
On leaving Uganda in 1955, the Ainsworths moved to Baltimore, where Mary Ainsworth
(?yg%~9o*m0began work as a diagnostician and part-time clinician at the Sheppard and Enoch Pratt Hospital, 心理学空间5[\|PQaC
further consolidating her already considerable assessment skills. At the same time, she taught
$~_Ug-jA_0B0clinical and developmental courses at the Johns Hopkins University, where she was initially hired
uSf!`pK/F0as a lecturer. Because of her involvement in diagnostic work and teaching, the data from the 心理学空间{8w;Gs6DdJ"h3P2I:E
Ganda project lay fallow for several years. 心理学空间g u*En2@y3A"[
心理学空间!gg`2v~9vmi5K pD

www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

«玛丽·爱因斯沃斯的陌生情境测验 安思沃斯 Ainsworth
《安思沃斯 Ainsworth》
没有了»